Published on in Vol 15 (2023)

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/50967, first published .
Intersection of Perceived COVID-19 Risk, Preparedness, and Preventive Health Behaviors: Latent Class Segmentation Analysis

Intersection of Perceived COVID-19 Risk, Preparedness, and Preventive Health Behaviors: Latent Class Segmentation Analysis

Intersection of Perceived COVID-19 Risk, Preparedness, and Preventive Health Behaviors: Latent Class Segmentation Analysis

Original Paper

1Institute of Community Health, University of Houston College of Pharmacy, Houston, TX, United States

2Department of Pharmaceutical Health Outcomes and Policy, University of Houston College of Pharmacy, Houston, TX, United States

3Public Health Science and Surveillance Division, Houston Health Department, Houston, TX, United States

4Department of Clinical Pharmacy and Outcomes Sciences, University of South Carolina College of Pharmacy, Columbia, SC, United States

5Department of Internal Medicine, General Hospital, Onitsha, Anambra State, Nigeria

Corresponding Author:

Osaro Mgbere, MS, MPH, PhD

Institute of Community Health

University of Houston College of Pharmacy

4349 Martin Luther King Blvd

Houston, TX, 77204

United States

Phone: 1 832 842 8393

Email: omgbere@uh.edu


Background: COVID-19 risk perception is a factor that influences the pandemic spread. Understanding the potential behavioral responses to COVID-19, including preparedness and adoption of preventive measures, can inform interventions to curtail its spread.

Objective: We assessed self-perceived and latent class analysis (LCA)–based risks of COVID-19 and their associations with preparedness, misconception, information gap, and preventive practices among residents of a densely populated city in Nigeria.

Methods: We used data from a cross-sectional survey conducted among residents (N=140) of Onitsha, Nigeria, in March 2020, before the government-mandated lockdown. Using an iterative expectation-maximization algorithm, we applied LCA to systematically segment participants into the most likely distinct risk clusters. Furthermore, we used bivariate and multivariable logistic regression models to determine the associations among knowledge, attitude, preventive practice, perceived preparedness, misconception, COVID-19 information gap, and self-perceived and LCA-based COVID-19 risks.

Results: Most participants (85/140, 60.7%) had good knowledge and did not perceive themselves as at risk of contracting COVID-19. Three-quarters of the participants (102/137, 74.6%; P<.001) experienced COVID-19–related information gaps, while 62.9% (88/140; P=.04) of the participants had some misconceptions about the disease. Conversely, most participants (93/140, 66.4%; P<.001) indicated that they were prepared for the COVID-19 pandemic. The majority of the participants (94/138, 68.1%; P<.001) self-perceived that they were not at risk of contracting COVID-19 compared to 31.9% (44/138) who professed to be at risk of contracting COVID-19. Using the LCA, we identified 3 distinct risk clusters (P<.001), namely, prudent or low-risk takers, skeptics or high-risk takers, and carefree or very high-risk takers with prevalence rates (probabilities of cluster membership that represent the prevalence rate [γc]) of 47.5% (95% CI 40%-55%), 16.2% (95% CI 11.4%-20.9%), and 36.4% (95% CI 28.8%-43.9%), respectively. We recorded a significantly negative agreement between self-perceived risk and LCA-based segmentation of COVID-19 risk (κ=–0.218, SD 0.067; P=.01). Knowledge, attitude, and perceived need for COVID-19 information were significant predictors of COVID-19 preventive practices among the Onitsha city residents.

Conclusions: The clustering patterns highlight the impact of modifiable risk behaviors on COVID-19 preventive practices, which can provide strong empirical support for health prevention policies. Consequently, clusters with individuals at high risk of contracting COVID-19 would benefit from multicomponent interventions delivered in diverse settings to improve the population-based response to the pandemic.

Online J Public Health Inform 2023;15:e50967

doi:10.2196/50967

Keywords



Since the declaration of COVID-19 as a public health emergency of international concern by the World Health Organization on January 30, 2020, the global case counts have reached 644 million, with more than 6.6 million deaths [1]. Despite recommended guidelines such as mask wearing, physical distancing, isolation, and good personal hygiene (handwashing) to prevent COVID-19 spread [2], some individuals tend to flout these guidelines, including government restrictions and rules. Government measures to contain the COVID-19 pandemic can only be effective with widespread compliance by the general public [3]. The fact that individuals in these challenging times act so differently indicates that the risk perception relating to this novel virus strongly differs between different places and individuals [4]. This situation, coupled with the pervasiveness of misinformation about the virus [5-9], is raising significant concerns, especially in Africa, where the fragile health systems put additional pressure on preparedness and effective pandemic response efforts. Consequently, COVID-19 risk perception may be a strong modifier of the evolution of the epidemic [4]. This means that access to and type of information received and political and economic situations may influence how people perceive the risk of COVID-19 and how they act on it, including their patterns of adhering to preventive actions [10].

In an early COVID-19 modeling study, Nigeria was identified as having a high coronavirus importation risk and high vulnerability, with moderate capacity to contain the outbreak [11]. The city of Onitsha in Nigeria is a highly populated town and home to the largest market in West Africa [12,13]. This city is known for the daily influx of people and frequent international, regional, and local interactions through commerce, which can facilitate the spread of COVID-19 within and outside the city. Previous studies in Onitsha have assessed COVID-19–related knowledge, attitude, and practice (KAP) [14]; public opinions regarding government response to the pandemic [3]; impact of COVID-19 misconceptions on the control efforts [15]; COVID-19–related information sources; and gaps [16]. However, it is unclear how the residents of Onitsha perceived the risk of COVID-19 and whether their initial risk perceptions informed their preparedness and decisions to adopt recommended protective measures. Risk perceptions tend to guide individuals’ judgments and evaluations of threats and can limit public compliance with and response to information from public health authorities [4,10,17,18].

Cori et al [4] espouse the need to apply established theories of risk perception research to COVID-19 and use this knowledge to improve health risk communication, build trust, and contribute to collaborating governance. People who perceive greater risks are believed to be more motivated to implement protective behaviors [10,17,18]. Although the global consequences of COVID-19 are hard to predict, an assessment of the initial population-level response to the disease in Onitsha regarding risk perception, preparedness, and preventive health behaviors could provide helpful information for interventions and improve current and future public health response. Understanding the risk perceptions of community residents is critical for planning, risk communication, and intervention.

In light of the foregoing situation, this study aims to (1) assess the association of self-perceived risk of COVID-19, preparedness, and preventive practices among residents of Onitsha, (2) apply latent class analysis (LCA) to systematically segment the heterogeneous sample population into the most likely distinct risk clusters by using selected measures, and (3) examine the predictive factors for COVID-19 preventive practices among Onitsha city residents. An understanding and application of the interplay of these factors in the real world could influence behavior change, improve risk management decision-making, and inform a more targeted and effective COVID-19 intervention strategy to prevent and control the disease spread in the city.


Data Source, Study Design, and Participants

We conducted a secondary analysis of cross-sectional data obtained from a KAP survey in Onitsha, Anambra, Nigeria, in March 2020 (period of the pandemic before the government-mandated lockdown on March 29, 2020) [14]. A convenience sampling method was used to recruit 140 study participants from different representative locations within the city of Onitsha that includes commercial markets and housing units. The city of Onitsha is the largest commercial city in south-eastern Nigeria and has a population of nearly 8.1 million residents with a population density of 4100 per square kilometer [19]. This survey was conducted through in-person interviews of consenting adults aged 18 years and older living and working in Onitsha. A more detailed description of the survey instrument used, data collection procedures, and the study area can be found in the study by Iloanusi et al [14].

Analytical Measures

The analytical measures used in this study were obtained from baseline data collection [14]. The data set captured KAP data as quantitative and categorical measures coded dichotomously as poor or good [14]. The quantitative part represented the KAP indices. COVID-19 misconceptions were assessed based on the study of Iloanusi et al [14] and categorized as none, low, and high [15]. Participants were asked to indicate what they considered to be their risk perception for COVID-19 by using a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from not at risk to extremely at risk). This was classified into 3 categories (not at risk at all, somewhat or likely at risk, likely or extremely likely at risk) and, subsequently, reclassified as 2 categories (not at risk vs at risk). To assess the participant’s perceived level of preparedness, they were asked how prepared they were for the impending COVID-19 outbreak, with the response options being “undecided or not prepared at all,” “somewhat prepared for COVID-19,” and “prepared for COVID-19.” This was later classified into 2 categories for subsequent analysis, with the last 2 options grouped as one and referred to as “prepared for COVID-19.” To assess information gaps, participants were asked if they needed COVID-19 information and coded dichotomously as Yes or No [14,17]. The demographic characteristics of the sample population used in this study have been described in detail previously by Iloanusi et al [14].

Statistical Analysis

Using the chi-square test, we conducted univariable analyses of measures of interest, namely, risk perception for COVID-19 outbreaks, KAP, participants’ level of preparedness, misconceptions, and the COVID-19 information gap. To examine the relationships between KAP indices by self-perceived COVID-19 risk, preparedness, and information (gap), we conducted a linear regression analysis with a density contour overlay to show the potential patterns (clusters) within the measures. The bivariate fit models produced equations that described the relationships between the measures.

LCA was used to fit a latent class model to determine the most likely cluster or latent class for each observable measure by using an iterative expectation-maximization algorithm [20,21]. LCA was considered appropriate for understanding and exploring the meaning behind the participants’ risk perception. Using LCA allowed for estimating the population characteristics derived from the sample data, adjusting the measurement error, and determining the number of classes [22]. The LCA produced the latent class prevalence (γc) and the conditional probabilities (ρ) for each cluster and response category. Estimates of the effect size and likelihood ratio logworth obtained from a contingency table analysis of expected counts for cluster membership by levels or categories of a Y column were used to quantify differences within the response scales. The final LCA model fitness was determined using negative log-likelihood (–log-likelihood), Bayesian information criterion, and Akaike information criterion to compare clusters with the smallest values of each, indicating the best fit. In addition, we considered entropy values, latent class probabilities, and interpretability of the model class identified in selecting the final model [21,23]. Based on these characteristics, definitions for each latent class were created. We identified 3 distinct risk clusters named as “prudent or low-risk takers,” “skeptics or high-risk takers,” and “carefree or very high-risk takers.” To enhance the ease of interpretation and clearer application to practice, the risk clusters were recoded dichotomously as “not at risk (0)” for prudent or low-risk takers and “at risk (1)” for both skeptics or high-risk takers and carefree or very high-risk takers. Furthermore, we determined the independent association between self-perceived COVID-19 risk and LCA-assessed COVID-19 risk and the study population characteristics by using a chi-square test or Fisher exact test (cell number <5), when applicable.

To identify factors associated with the adoption of COVID-19 preventive practice, we conducted multivariable logistic regression analyses to estimate the unadjusted odds ratios and adjusted odds ratios (aORs) along with 95% CIs and the corresponding P values for each factor. We applied the mosaic plot data visualization with the associated chi-square test to display the intersection between self-perceived COVID-19 risk, LCA-based risk subgroup, and preventive practices by perceived preparedness. All statistical tests performed were 2-tailed, with a probability value of .05 used as the minimum threshold for declaring statistical significance. Data management, statistical analyses, and visualizations were conducted using SAS JMP Statistical Discovery Software (version 16.2; SAS Institute). This study was reported following the STROBE (Strengthening The Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology) statement [24] and aligns with the minimum specific reporting requirements in the cross-sectional study checklist [25].

Ethics Approval

All relevant ethical guidelines, including institutional review board approval and oral informed consent, were provided by all participants and documented during the primary data collection period [14]. Data used for this study were codified and anonymized to protect confidentiality and ensure individual participants' privacy. The study protocol for this secondary data analysis was reviewed and approved (approval 00002363) by the institutional review board of the University of Houston.


Descriptive Analysis of Measures

The descriptive characteristics of the study participants have been presented in detail by Iloanusi et al [14]. The demographic characteristics of this study population is displayed in Multimedia Appendix 1. The univariable analysis of the measures evaluated is shown in Table 1. We noted a significant difference in participants’ COVID-19 knowledge levels, with more than half of them (85/140, 60.7%; P=.01) having a high knowledge of the disease. However, the majority (85/140, 60.7%; P<.001) did not perceive they were at risk of contracting COVID-19. About 62.9% (88/140; P=.047) of the participants had some misconceptions about COVID-19, while only 37.1% (52/140; P=.047) had no misconceptions about the disease. As much as 74.6% (102/137; P<.001) indicated the need for more COVID-19–related information. However, most participants (93/140, 66.4%; P<.001) indicated that they were prepared for the COVID-19 pandemic. Overall, the study participants (N=140) were indifferent (P>.05) in their attitude and preventive practice levels.

Table 1. Univariable analysis of the study measures (N=140).
CharacteristicValues, n (%)P value
Knowledge.01a

Low55 (39.3)

High85 (60.7)
Attitude.13b

Poor61 (43.6)

Good79 (56.4)
Preventive practice.39b

Poor65 (46.4)

Good75 (53.6)
COVID-19 misconception.047c

None52 (37.1)

Low55 (39.3)

High33 (23.6)
Perceived need for COVID-19 information<.001d

No35 (25.5)

Yes102 (74.5)
Perceived preparedness<.001d

Not prepared for COVID-1947 (33.6)

Prepared for COVID-1993 (66.4)

aSignificant at P<.01.

bNot significant (P>.05).

cSignificant at P<.05.

dSignificant at P<.001.

Relationships Between KAP Indices

The relationships among KAP indices by COVID-19 risk perception are shown in Figure 1A. A linear relationship was observed across the measures among individuals who perceived that they were at risk or not at risk of COVID-19. The contours show the regions of data density relative to the indices. Among the at-risk group, increased COVID-19 knowledge index and attitude index scores resulted in an increased level of preventive practice implementation (R2=0.206 for knowledge index; P<.001 vs R2=0.107 for attitude index; P=.05). In contrast, knowledge index and attitude index significantly (P<.001) predicted as much as 52.7% and 29.2% of the preventive practice levels implemented by study participants who perceived they were not at risk of contracting COVID-19.

Figure 1B depicts the relationships among KAP indices by perceived COVID-19 information gap with the bands indicating the concentration of participants relative to their scores. Within participants who perceived that they had gaps in COVID-19–related information, increased knowledge index and attitude index resulted in a significant linear increase in their adoption of recommended preventive practices, as indicated by the preventive practice indices (R2=0.359 vs R2=0.193, respectively). However, participants who did not perceive any gap in COVID-19 information had higher predictive associations among KAP indices. For instance, a positive increase in knowledge index and attitude index resulted in a corresponding increase in preventive practice indices and coefficients of determination of 60.8% (knowledge index, P<.001) and 20.9% (attitude index, P<.01).

The relationships among KAP indices by perceived preparedness with the contours indicating the density of participants relative to their index values are shown in Figure 1C. Within participants who perceived that they were prepared for the COVID-19 pandemic, increased knowledge index and attitude index brought about a significant (P<.001) linear increase in their adoption of recommended preventive practices, as reflected in the preventive practice index values (R2=0.378 vs R2=0.151, respectively). However, participants who perceived themselves unprepared for the COVID-19 pandemic recorded comparatively higher predictive values. For instance, a positive increase in knowledge index and attitude index resulted in a corresponding increase in preventive practice index and coefficients of determination of 55.5% (knowledge index, P<.001) and 37.5% (attitude index, P<.001).

Figure 1. (A) Relationships among knowledge, attitude, and preventive practice indices by self-perceived COVID-19 risk. (B) Relationships among knowledge, attitude, and preventive practice indices by perceived COVID-19 information gap. (C) Relationships among knowledge, attitude, and preventive practice indices by perceived COVID-19 pandemic preparedness.

LCA Model

The LCA model parameter estimates with the conditional probabilities (ρ) for each cluster and response category are presented in Table 2. Following the study population heterogeneity concerning a set of manifest variables, we used LCA to identify 3 significantly (P<.001) distinct most likely clusters or latent classes (homogeneous subgroups or segments) of individuals. Those were named after their unique characteristics as prudent or low-risk takers (γ=47.5%), skeptics or high-risk takers (γ=16.2%), and carefree or very high-risk takers (γ=36.4%). The key characteristics of the prudent or low-risk takers include high COVID-19 knowledge (ρ=95.2%), good attitude (ρ=85.4%), self-perception of not being at risk of COVID-19 (ρ=58.9%), perceived preparedness for COVID-19 pandemic (ρ=62%), experienced information gap (ρ=81.5%), no misconceptions (ρ=66.5%), and good preventive practices (ρ=87.5%). Among individuals characterized as skeptics or high-risk takers, 59.9% of them had high knowledge of COVID-19, a poor attitude (ρ=66.6%), claimed not to be at risk of COVID-19 (ρ=54.9%), perceived themselves as being prepared for the disease (ρ=87%), had very serious information gaps (ρ=98.5%), many misconceptions (ρ=97.9%), and surprisingly, practiced good prevention strategies against COVID-19 (ρ=82.5%). However, carefree or very high-risk takers had low knowledge (ρ=81.3%), poor attitude (ρ=72.7%), self-perception of not being at risk of COVID-19 (ρ=84.5%), perceived preparedness for COVID-19 pandemic (ρ=60%), experienced information gap (ρ=53.5%), had moderate misconceptions (ρ=68.1%), and implemented poor preventive practices (ρ=99.4%).

Table 2. Parameter estimates from latent class segmentation analysis.a
Parameter, categorynProbability (ρ) of latent class membership (%)Effect sizeLRLb


Prudent or low-risk takersSkeptics or high-risk takersCarefree or very high-risk takers

Overall (γc)14047.516.236.4 —c
Prevention practice0.83d24.80

Poor6512.517.599.4


Good7587.582.50.6

Knowledge0.71d17.16

Low554.840.181.3


High8595.259.918.7

Attitude0.57e10.20

Poor6114.666.672.7


Good7985.433.427.3

Risk perception0.28f2.39

Not at risk of COVID-199458.954.984.5


At risk of COVID-194441.145.115.5

Perceived preparedness0.20f1.36

Not prepared for COVID-1947381340


Prepared for COVID-1991628760

COVID-19 information gap0.38f4.81

Yes10181.598.553.5


No3518.51.546.5

Misconception0.48e8.15

No5266.52.131.9


Yes8833.597.968.1

aThe overall probabilities of cluster membership (γc) and the conditional probabilities (ρ) for each cluster are shown for each response category. Model fit statistics best fit estimates: negative log-likelihood=563.04; Bayesian information criterion=1239.07; and Akaike information criterion=1172.08.

bLRL: likelihood ratio logworth. A logworth value above 2 corresponds to significance at the .01 significance level (P<.01).

cNot applicable.

dLarge effect size.

eMedium effect size.

fSmall effect size.

Associations Between Self-Perceived and LCA-Based Segmentation of COVID-19 Risks and Selected Measures

Table 3 shows the associations among KAP, misconception, perceived information needs, preparedness, COVID-19 risk perception, and LCA COVID-19 risk assessment. Most participants (94/138, 68.1%; P<.001) self-perceived that they were not at risk of COVID-19 compared to 31.9% (44/138; P<.001) who professed to be at risk of contracting COVID-19. Based on the LCA assessment, we recorded no statistical difference (P>.05) between the 2 groups (67/136, 49.3% vs 69/136, 50.7%). However, with the LCA classification, we recorded statistically significant variations (P=.05) across all measures considered, except perceived preparedness compared to self-perceived risk assessment, where knowledge (P=.01) and preventive practice (P=.03) were the only significant sources of variations. Based on the participant’s characteristics, the LCA generally tends to classify more participants as at risk of contracting COVID-19 than the self-perceived assessment. For instance, the proportion of participants who self-perceived themselves as being at risk were 5, 3, 3.8, and 3 times more than those who had poor COVID-19 knowledge, attitude, preventive practice, and high misconception, respectively, based on the LCA-based assessment of their COVID-19 risk status.

Table 3. Associations between self-perceived and latent class analysis–based segmentation of COVID-19 risk and selected measures.
MeasureSelf-perceived COVID-19 risk (n=138)Latent class analysis–based COVID-19 risk assessment (n=136)

Not at risk, n (%)At risk, n (%)P valueNot at risk, n (%)At risk, n (%)P value
Overall94 (68.1)44 (31.9)<.001a67 (49.3)69 (50.7).86b
Knowledge.01c

<.001a

Low43 (31.2)10 (7.3)
2 (1.5)50 (36.8)

High51 (37)34 (24.6)
65 (47.8)19 (13.9)
Attitude.68b

<.001a

Poor42 (30.4)18 (13)
7 (5.2)53 (38.9)

Good52 (37.7)26 (18.8)
60 (44.1)16 (11.8)
Preventive practice.03d

<.001a

Poor49 (35.5)14 (10.1)
9 (6.6)52 (38.2)

Good45 (32.6)30 (21.7)
58 (42.7)17 (12.5)
COVID-19 misconception.96b

<.001a

None34 (24.6)17 (12.3)
37 (27.2)13 (9.6)

Low37 (26.8)17 (12.3)
27 (19.8)26 (19.1)

High23 (16.7)10 (7.3)
3 (2.2)30 (22.1)
Perceived need for COVID-19 information.33b

.045d

No26 (19.1)9 (6.6)
54 (40)46 (34.1)

Yes66 (48.5)35 (25.7)
12 (8.9)23 (17)
Perceived preparedness.44b

.59b

Not prepared for COVID-1934 (24.6)13 (9.4)
25 (18.4)22 (16.2)

Prepared for COVID-1960 (43.5)31 (22.5)
42 (30.8)47 (34.6)

aSignificant at P<.001.

bNot significant (P>.05).

cSignificant at P<.01.

dSignificant at P<.05.

Multivariable Logistic Regression Model

The multivariable logistic regression model showing the unadjusted and aORs of the measures and COVID-19 preventive practice is presented in Table 4. Of all the measures included in our model, knowledge, attitude, and perceived need for COVID-19 information were the participants’ only significant predictors of COVID-19 preventive practices (entropy R2=0.3352). Participants who had high COVID-19 knowledge and good attitude toward the management of COVID-19 were 11 (aOR 11.22, 95% CI 4.34-28.97; P<.001) and 3 (aOR 2.93, 95% CI 1.14-7.55; P=.03) times more likely to have good COVID-19 preventative practices, respectively. Surprisingly, participants who needed more COVID-19 information were about 4 times more likely (aOR 3.92, 95% CI 1.36-11.30; P<.01) to have good COVID-19 preventive practices compared to those who experienced no information gap.

Table 4. Multivariable logistic regression model of COVID-19 preventive practice.a
MeasuresUnadjustedAdjusted

Odds ratio (95% CI)P valueOdds ratio (95% CI)P value
Knowledge<.001b
<.001b

Low (reference)1
1

High14.63 (6.26-34.18)
11.22 (4.34-28.97)
Attitude<.001b
.03c

Poor (reference)1
1

Good4.70 (2.29-9.63)
2.93 (1.14-7.55)
Risk perception for COVID-19.03c
.41d

Not at risk at all (reference)1
1

At risk2.33 (1.10-4.95)
1.51 (0.57-3.99)
COVID-19 misconception.14d
.99d

Low (reference)1
1

High0.60 (0.85-3.27)
0.99 (0.37-2.65)
Perceived need for COVID-19 information.01b
.01e

No (reference)1
1

Yes4.21 (1.82-9.71)
3.92 (1.36-11.30)
Perceived preparedness.67d
.85d

Not prepared for COVID-19 (reference)1
1

Prepared for COVID-191.16 (0.58-2.35)
1.10 (0.41-2.92)

aModel statistics: McFadden’s pseudo R-square (R2 [U])=0.34; Akaike information criterion=139.25; Bayesian information criterion=158.76. Normal approximation used for ratio confidence limits effects. Tests and confidence intervals of odds ratios are Wald test–based.

bSignificant at P<.001.

cSignificant at P<.05.

dNot significant (P>.05).

eSignificant at P<.01.

Intersection of Perceived COVID-19 Risk, Latent Class Risk Subgroup, Preparedness, and Preventive Practice

The mosaic plot in Figure 2 displays the associations between self-perceived COVID-19 risk, LCA-based risk subgroup, and preventive practices by perceived preparedness. Among participants who perceived themselves as not at risk of COVID-19 but were prepared, 71% (5/7) were characterized as prudent low-risk takers and adopted good preventive strategies (P<.001). Similarly, most of the participants who perceived themselves as somewhat likely or likely or extremely likely at risk of contracting COVID-19 and were prepared for the pandemic ended up as prudent low-risk takers (5/7, 71%; P=.05 vs 12/16, 75%; P=.01, respectively) and implemented good preventive measures. However, most participants, including those who were prepared and not prepared for the COVID-19 pandemic, practiced the recommended preventive measures poorly (60%-100%) and were classified as carefree or very high-risk takers based on the LCA model. Only 29% (2/7; P=.05) and 25% (4/16; P=.01) of the participants who implemented good preventive practices were skeptics or high-risk takers and self-perceived themselves as likely at risk and extremely likely at risk of contracting COVID-19 (Figure 2). Overall, we recorded a significant negative agreement between self-perceived and LCA-based segmentation of the risk groups (κ=–0.2182, SD 0.0665; P=.01).

Figure 2. Mosaic plot of the associations between self-perceived COVID-19 risk and latent class analysis–based risk assessment of most likely cluster membership by perceived preparedness.

Principal Findings

During the initial days of the COVID-19 pandemic, the Nigerian government, like many other governments, implemented a series of preventive practices that included public lockdown, handwashing, use of hand sanitizer, wearing of face masks, and social distancing in public places, to curtail the spread of COVID-19. Although these interventions are effective, they require voluntary behavior change and compliance by citizens, who found this challenging to achieve and monitor. Our study investigates the role of behavioral determinants on self-perceived and LCA-based COVID-19 risks and the adoption of COVID-19 preventive practices. Our findings revealed that adopting good COVID-19 preventive practices among residents of Onitsha was generally driven by increased knowledge and improved attitude toward COVID-19 infection moderated by their risk perception status, information gaps, and level of preparedness. Several COVID-19–related studies have previously established the relationships between COVID-19 knowledge, attitude, and prevention practices [14,26,27]. Individuals who believed they were at low risk of developing COVID-19 were more likely to engage in unhealthy or risky behaviors [28].

Using the LCA, we identified 3 distinct risk clusters (P<.001), namely, prudent or low-risk takers, skeptics or high-risk takers, and carefree or very high-risk takers, with prevalence rates of 47.5%, 16.2%, and 36.4%, respectively. Individuals who were high-risk to very high–risk takers tended to have many misconceptions, experienced COVID-19 information gaps, self-perceived themselves as not being at risk of contracting COVID-19, and therefore adopted poor preventive practices. A previous study has highlighted the existence of fundamental misconceptions that hindered compliance with prevention practices among Onitsha residents [15]. Similarly, COVID-19 information gaps and low diffusion due to government delays in initiating awareness campaigns using accessible and efficient channels of information have been associated with poor COVID-19 preventive practices [16,29,30].

Accurate public risk perceptions are critical to effectively managing COVID-19, especially considering that people’s behavior can fundamentally influence and alter the spread of a pandemic [31-33]. In an attempt to assess “COVID-19 risk as feelings” (self-perceived) and “COVID-19 risk as analysis” (LCA-based assessment), we recorded a significant negative agreement between self-perceived risk and LCA-based segmentation of COVID-19 risk (κ=–0.218, SD 0.067; P<.01). Consequently, participants with poor knowledge, attitude, preventive practice, and high misconception who claimed not to be at risk of COVID-19 infection were reported to be 3-5 times more at risk of contracting COVID-19 when assessed using LCA. These measures also had significant effect sizes in determining the probability of latent class membership. Although risk perceptions influence individual protective behaviors [17,34], our findings indicate that an individual’s perception of risk may not necessarily correlate positively with the actual analyzed risk. A pessimistic bias, that is, perceptions of risk that are (much) higher than the actual risk, is more likely for new risks such as COVID-19 that are perceived as uncontrollable [35].

LCA was considered appropriate for understanding and exploring the meaning behind risk perceptions in our study population. Threat appraisal and risk perception are essential determinants of the public’s willingness to cooperate and adopt health-protective behaviors during pandemics, including frequent handwashing, physical distancing, avoiding public places, and wearing face masks [36-38]. These risk perceptions guide individuals’ judgments and evaluations of threats and can limit public compliance with and response to information that authorities communicate [4,10,17,18,39]. LCA has been used to study various issues in vulnerable populations, including mental health among Black youth [40], young Malawian adults with or at risk for HIV [41], and adolescent perceptions of in-school discrimination [42].

Of all the measures included in our multivariable model, only knowledge, attitude, and perceived need for COVID-19 information were significant predictors of COVID-19 preventive practices among the participants. Previous studies have also documented the associations between the KAP indices in this population [14]. However, it was surprising that participants who experienced the COVID-19 information gap were about 4 times more likely to have good preventative practices against the disease. This suggests that public health messages by the Nigeria Center for Disease Control and other governmental agencies may have been responsible for a positive behavioral change toward risk aversion during the early stage of the COVID-19 pandemic [3,16]. Residents of Onitsha, like many others worldwide, were faced with a new and unfamiliar health threat that could result in deaths, coupled with the fact that information on the disease was initially limited and changed more often with time. Fear of the unknown has been associated with the absence of information and, when encountered in sufficiently predictable and controllable contexts, could facilitate positive responses [43,44]. This may have been the case in our study population.

The intersection of self-perceived COVID-19 risk, LCA most likely risk clusters, perceived preparedness, and preventive practice indicates that most participants who self-perceived themselves as not being at risk of COVID-19 and had poor preventive practices (carefree ones) were noted to be at a very high risk of contracting COVID-19 infection when assessed using LCA. This make-belief may have brought about complacency on the public part, enforced by false or misleading COVID-19 narratives promoted by some groups to discredit legitimate public health measures [45]. The advent of social media and web-based platforms, which provide a fertile medium for disinformation to flourish, has been widely acknowledged as a threat to global efforts toward ending the pandemic [15,16,40]. This situation raises important concerns, especially in Africa, where the fragile health systems put additional pressure on preparedness and effective pandemic response efforts. However, our study noted that all the participants who claimed nonpreparedness but the likelihood of being somewhat at risk of COVID-19 accurately matched the LCA-based assessment classification as prudent or low-risk takers. People who perceive greater risks are believed to be more motivated to implement protective behaviors [10,17,18]. Efforts to improve pandemic preparedness and response to the next pandemic might benefit from greater investment in risk communication and community engagement and in developing strategies to counter misinformation and boost individuals' confidence in public health guidance [15,16,39,44-46]. Since latent class membership helps explain the patterns of individuals’ scores on the indicator variables used to derive the classes, the LCA solutions therefore represent typologies that can help researchers and practitioners understand commonalities and differences across individuals, which have implications for both practice and future research [21].

Strengths and Limitations

This study’s findings should be interpreted with caution because of some limitations. This study is a secondary data analysis based on a cross-sectional study conducted during the early onset of the COVID-19 pandemic using a nonprobability convenience sampling method with a small sample size. Therefore, the data may be subject to sampling and potential response biases due to social desirability and unobserved confounding, leading to nonrepresentativeness of the population and possibly overestimating the direction and strength of associations. Similarly, the use of LCA is limited by the fact that individuals are assigned to classes based on the probability of likely cluster membership predicated on the scores of the indicator variables [21]. As a result, the exact number or percentage of class memberships cannot be guaranteed due to some level of misclassifications. Further, the complexity of the classes may inadvertently engage in a naming fallacy, wherein the class name does not accurately reflect the class membership [21]. Therefore, from the foregoing situation, definite causality cannot be inferred, and the generalizability of our findings is limited. The government intervention’s role in messaging may also have impacted an individual’s risk assessment and response to preventive care practices [3]. Finally, it is essential to note that the availability and uptake of the COVID-19 vaccines more than 2 years after the data used in this study were collected may have decreased self-perceived risk and adherence to COVID-19 preventive measures over time [47].

Despite these limitations, our study’s strength lies in applying latent class segmentation analysis to reveal important insights into the relationships between behavioral measures and COVID-19 infection risk. Although self-report may not allow for the assessment of actual behavior due to social desirability, the application of LCA allowed for culturally competent and context-specific risk classification, which may be particularly useful in identifying subgroups of individuals who could benefit from a common intervention based on their shared characteristics [21,23]. Data used for this study were collected during the early phase of the COVID-19 pandemic and, thus, provide rich baseline information that could be used by public health authorities to assess COVID-19 response efforts or for current and future pandemic intervention planning.

Conclusion

The LCA clustering patterns highlight the impact of modifiable risk behaviors on COVID-19 preventive practices, which can provide strong empirical support that may encourage behavior change, especially during the COVID-19 pandemic or any future outbreak of similar infectious diseases. Consequently, clusters with individuals at high risk of contracting COVID-19 would benefit from multicomponent interventions delivered in diverse settings to improve the population response to the COVID-19 pandemic. This finding may also offer clinicians the opportunity to refer their patients at high risk of contracting the disease to social workers or psychologists for behavioral counseling. In addition, understanding the role of risk perceptions in motivating people to engage in preventive behavior by public health authorities may also help with intervention program planning and designing evidence-based risk communication strategies.

Data Availability

Data for this study can be made available upon reasonable request from the corresponding author.

Authors' Contributions

OM contributed to conceptualization, project administration, data curation, methodology, formal analysis, validation, visualization, writing the original draft, reviewing, and editing. SI contributed to conceptualization, project administration, resources, methodology, data curation, validation, reviewing, and editing. IY, N-JRI, SG, and EJE contributed to validation, reviewing, and editing. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript submitted for publication.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

Multimedia Appendix 1

Demographic characteristics of the study population.

PNG File , 37 KB

  1. WHO. WHO coronavirus disease (COVID-19) dashboard. WHO. Nov 3, 2020;10(1):1-3. [CrossRef]
  2. How to protect yourself and others. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. May 08, 2020. URL: https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/prevent-getting-sick/prevention.html [accessed 2023-09-26]
  3. Yunusa I, Iloanusi S, Mgbere O, Iloanusi NR, Ajayi AI, Essien EJ. Public opinion regarding government response to COVID-19: case study of a large commercial city in Nigeria. Pan Afr Med J. 2021;38:282. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  4. Cori L, Bianchi F, Cadum E, Anthonj C. Risk perception and COVID-19. Int J Environ Res Public Health. Apr 29, 2020;17(9):3114. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Munich security conference. WHO. 2020. URL: https://www.who.int/dg/speeches/detail/munich-security-conference [accessed 2020-03-30]
  6. Chou WS, Oh A, Klein WMP. Addressing health-related misinformation on social media. JAMA. Dec 18, 2018;320(23):2417-2418. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Moorhead SA, Hazlett DE, Harrison L, Carroll JK, Irwin A, Hoving C. A new dimension of health care: systematic review of the uses, benefits, and limitations of social media for health communication. J Med Internet Res. Apr 23, 2013;15(4):e85. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. The Lancet. COVID-19: fighting panic with information. The Lancet. Feb 2020;395(10224):537. [CrossRef]
  9. Mohammed M, Sha'aban A, Jatau AI, Yunusa I, Isa AM, Wada AS, et al. Assessment of COVID-19 information overload among the general public. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. Feb 2022;9(1):184-192. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Nshakira-Rukundo E, Whitehead A. Changing perceptions about COVID-19 risk and adherence to preventive strategies in Uganda: Evidence from an online mixed-methods survey. Sci Afr. Nov 2021;14:e01049. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  11. Gilbert M, Pullano G, Pinotti F, Valdano E, Poletto C, Boëlle P, et al. Preparedness and vulnerability of African countries against importations of COVID-19: a modelling study. The Lancet. Mar 2020;395(10227):871-877. [CrossRef]
  12. Lady-Franca OC. Induction strategy of Igbo entrepreneurs and micro-business success: A study of household equipment line, main market Onitsha, Nigeria. Acta Universitatis Sapientiae, Economics and Business. (1). 2016;4:43-65. [CrossRef]
  13. Onitsha. Wikipedia. URL: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Onitsha [accessed 2021-11-17]
  14. Iloanusi NR, Iloanusi S, Mgbere O, Ajayi A, Essien EJ. COVID-19 related knowledge, attitude and practices in a Southeastern City in Nigeria: A cross-sectional survey. SSRN Journal. Sep 15, 2020:1-24. [CrossRef]
  15. Iloanusi S, Mgbere O, Iloanusi NR, Yunusa I, Essien EJ. COVID-19 related misconceptions and prevention practices among the public in a southeastern city in Nigeria. Int J Transl Med Res Public Health. Aug 14, 2021;5(2):149-159. [CrossRef]
  16. Iloanusi N, Iloanusi S, Mgbere O, Ajayi A, Essien E. PIN34 COVID-19 related knowledge, attitude and practices in a southeastern city in Nigeria: a cross-sectional survey. Value in Health. Jun 2021;24:S111-S112. [CrossRef]
  17. Bruine de Bruin W, Bennett D. Relationships between initial COVID-19 risk perceptions and protective health behaviors: a national survey. Am J Prev Med. Aug 2020;59(2):157-167. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. Fischhoff B. Risk perception communication. In: Risk Analysis and Human Behavior. London, UK. Routledge; 2012.
  19. Demographia world urban areas. Demographia. 2019. URL: http://www.demographia.com/db-worldua.pdf [accessed 2023-09-26]
  20. Collins LM, Lanza ST. Latent Class and Latent Transition Analysis: With Applications in the Social, Behavioral, and Health Sciences. 111 River St, USA. Wiley; 2010.
  21. Weller BE, Bowen NK, Faubert SJ. Latent class analysis: A guide to best practice. Journal of Black Psychology. Jun 05, 2020;46(4):287-311. [CrossRef]
  22. Kukreti S, Yu T, Chiu PW, Strong C. Clustering of modifiable behavioral risk factors and their association with all-cause mortality in Taiwan's adult population: a latent class analysis. Int J Behav Med. Oct 2022;29(5):565-574. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  23. Muthen B, Muthen L. Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res. Jun 2000;24(6):882-891. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
  24. von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP, et al. STROBE Initiative. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Int J Surg. Dec 2014;12(12):1495-1499. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. STROBE statement—Checklist of items that should be included in reports of cross-sectional studies. Equator-Network. URL: https://www.equator-network.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/10/STROBE_checklist_v4_cross-sectional.pdf [accessed 2023-02-13]
  26. Austrian K, Pinchoff J, Tidwell JB, White C, Abuya T, Kangwana B, et al. COVID-19-related knowledge, attitudes, practices and needs of households in informal settlements in Nairobi, Kenya. Lancet Global Health. 2020:2020. [CrossRef]
  27. Zhong B, Luo W, Li H, Zhang Q, Liu X, Li W, et al. Knowledge, attitudes, and practices towards COVID-19 among Chinese residents during the rapid rise period of the COVID-19 outbreak: a quick online cross-sectional survey. Int J Biol Sci. 2020;16(10):1745-1752. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Jose R, Narendran M, Bindu A, Beevi N, Benny PV. Public perception and preparedness for the pandemic COVID 19: A Health Belief Model approach. Clin Epidemiol Glob Health. 2021;9:41-46. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. Lin S, Ma C, Lin R, Huang J, Xu R, Yuan A. Research on the influence of information diffusion on the transmission of the novel coronavirus (COVID-19). medRxiv. Preprint posted online on April 2, 2020. [CrossRef]
  30. Zhan X, Liu C, Zhou G, Zhang Z, Sun G, Zhu JJ, et al. Coupling dynamics of epidemic spreading and information diffusion on complex networks. Appl Math Comput. Sep 01, 2018;332:437-448. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  31. Funk S, Gilad E, Watkins C, Jansen VAA. The spread of awareness and its impact on epidemic outbreaks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. Apr 21, 2009;106(16):6872-6877. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  32. Reluga TC. Game theory of social distancing in response to an epidemic. PLoS Comput Biol. May 27, 2010;6(5):e1000793. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  33. Bavel JJV, Baicker K, Boggio PS, Capraro V, Cichocka A, Cikara M, et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat Hum Behav. May 2020;4(5):460-471. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  34. van der Pligt J. Risk perception and self-protective behavior. European Psychologist. Jan 1996;1(1):34-43. [CrossRef]
  35. de Zwart O, Veldhuijzen IK, Elam G, Aro AR, Abraham T, Bishop GD, et al. Perceived threat, risk perception, and efficacy beliefs related to SARS and other (emerging) infectious diseases: results of an international survey. Int J Behav Med. 2009;16(1):30-40. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  36. Poletti P, Ajelli M, Merler S. The effect of risk perception on the 2009 H1N1 pandemic influenza dynamics. PLoS One. Feb 07, 2011;6(2):e16460. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  37. van der Weerd W, Timmermans DR, Beaujean DJ, Oudhoff J, van Steenbergen JE. Monitoring the level of government trust, risk perception and intention of the general public to adopt protective measures during the influenza A (H1N1) pandemic in The Netherlands. BMC Public Health. Jul 19, 2011;11:575. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  38. Rudisill C. How do we handle new health risks? Risk perception, optimism, and behaviors regarding the H1N1 virus. Journal of Risk Research. Sep 2013;16(8):959-980. [CrossRef]
  39. Anna-Leena L, Jussi S, Katja S, Taneli P, Mika S. Understanding coronavirus disease (COVID-19) risk perceptions among the public to enhance risk communication efforts: a practical approach for outbreaks, Finland, February 2020. Euro Surveill. 2020;25(13):a. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
  40. Rose T, Lindsey MA, Xiao Y, Finigan-Carr NM, Joe S. Mental health and educational experiences among Black youth: A latent class analysis. J Youth Adolesc. Nov 2017;46(11):2321-2340. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  41. Weller B, Small E. Profiles of risk for HIV/AIDS among young Malawian adults: understanding behavioral intentions. Sexuality & Culture. May 15, 2015;19(4):685-696. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
  42. Byrd C, Carter Andrews DJ. Variations in students' perceived reasons for, sources of, and forms of in-school discrimination: A latent class analysis. J Sch Psychol. Aug 2016;57:1-14. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  43. Carleton RN. Into the unknown: A review and synthesis of contemporary models involving uncertainty. J Anxiety Disord. Apr 2016;39:30-43. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Rubin GJ, Amlôt R, Page L, Wessely S. Public perceptions, anxiety, and behaviour change in relation to the swine flu outbreak: cross sectional telephone survey. BMJ. Jul 02, 2009;339:b2651. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  45. Wang Y, Bye J, Bales K, Gurdasani D, Mehta A, Abba-Aji M, et al. Understanding and neutralising COVID-19 misinformation and disinformation. BMJ. Nov 22, 2022;379:e070331. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  46. Edwards AM, Baric RS, Saphire EO, Ulmer JB. Stopping pandemics before they start: Lessons learned from SARS-CoV-2. Science. Mar 11, 2022;375(6585):1133-1139. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  47. Nicolò VD, Villari P, Vito CD. Impact of COVID-19 vaccination on risk perception: a cross-sectional study on vaccinated people. European Journal of Public Health. Oct 03, 2022;32(3):ckac129.664. [CrossRef]


aOR: adjusted odds ratio
KAP: knowledge, attitude, and practice
LCA: latent class analysis
STROBE: Strengthening The Reporting of Observational studies in Epidemiology


Edited by T Leung, E Mensah; submitted 18.07.23; peer-reviewed by YH Li, P Masurka; comments to author 18.07.23; revised version received 13.08.23; accepted 07.09.23; published 24.10.23.

Copyright

©Osaro Mgbere, Sorochi Iloanusi, Ismaeel Yunusa, Nchebe-Jah R Iloanusi, Shrey Gohil, Ekere James Essien. Originally published in the Online Journal of Public Health Informatics (https://ojphi.jmir.org/), 24.10.2023.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in the Online Journal of Public Health Informatics, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://ojphi.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.